Ryszard Praszkier The empassion scale: introduction, validation, and application

PDF Abstrakt

Rocznik: 2023

Tom: XXVIII

Numer: 1

Tytuł: The empassion scale: introduction, validation, and application

Autorzy: Ryszard Praszkier

PFP: 37–55

DOI: https://doi.org/10.34767/PFP.2023.01.03

Artykuł jest dostępny na warunkach międzynarodowej licencji 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0).

*Acknowledgements. Thank you to Paige Munnik for her significant editorial contribution. This article is assigned to the Robert B. Zajonc Institute for Social Studies, University of Warsaw. 

Declarations
Funding: This research did not receive any specific grant from any funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Conflicts of Interest: The author declares that there were no conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship or the publication of this article.
Ethics Statement: Data collection of this study was reviewed and accepted by the Research Ethics Committee, Institute for Social Science, University of Warsaw.
Data Availability: The data for this study are available from the author upon request.

Introduction: Empathy and Compassion

It was Aristotle (4th century BC) who proposed the term philia as the relationship between people who feel philēesis (mutual understanding) for one another. In Aristotle’s understanding, philēesis is a deep desire for the eudaimonia of another person—most closely explained as empathy and compassion (Curzer, 2007; Barnes, 2014).

In the Middle Ages, Augustine (5th century AC) argued that compassion is the ability to feel another’s misery, which encourages us to provide support (Ruys, 2018). Some authors claim that in the Middle Ages, compassion and empathy played a significant role, despite not yet having been named or fully discovered (e.g., Morrison, 2013).

At the beginning of the 20th century, John Dewey considered community and civil society as the social core, highlighting mutual care and understanding, which today is apparently perceived as social compassion and empathy (Dewey, 1966).

Compassion has always been present in religious teachings, and—together with empathy—is a fulcrum for positive social relationships (Davis, 2017).

Empathy

At the beginning of the 20th century, German psychologist Theodor Lipps introduced the term Einfühlung (German: “feeling with”), referring to the tendency of perceivers to project themselves into the objects of perception and experience themselves as being “in” the object (Lipps, 1903; Håkansson, 2003; Praszkier, 2014).

The term empathy per se was coined by American psychologist Edward Bradford Titchener during the process of developing Lipp’s concept (Titchener, 1909).

The empathy concept for humans is essential in many fields, e.g., social and personality psychology, neuroscience, and clinical psychology (Hall, Schwartz, 2017). However, in the literature, multiple denotations of this concept are available, e.g., Cuff et al. identified 43 definitions (2014, pp. 3–4).

Here, empathy is defined as the proficiency to understand or feel what another person is experiencing, from their point of view; in other words, it is the ability to place oneself in another person’s shoes (Lazarus, 1994; Davis, 2006; Barnett, Mann, 2013; Praszkier, 2014; Breyer, 2020).

There are two basic dimensions of empathy (Cuff et al., 2014; Read, 2019): affective, i.e., emotionally tuning into the feelings of others (Batson et al., 2005; Decety, Lamm, 2006; Eisenberg, Strayer, 1987; Hein, Singer, 2008; Roy, 2010), and cognitive, i.e., the ability to understand another (Mead, 1967; Ickes, 1997; De Vignemont, Singer, 2006; Piaget, 2008; van der Weele, 2011). Some definitions, however, include both (Cohen, Strayer, 1996; Batson et al., 2005; Eisenberg, Fabes, Spinrad, 2006; Decety, Moriguchi, 2007; Oliveira-Silva, Gonçalves, 2011).

Compassion

There is indication that compassion has been prevalent among animals, especially humanoids, as since prehistoric times (Hublin, 2009).

In contemporary times, compassion directly addresses caring for others. It is defined as the capacity for being moved by the suffering of others and wanting to help alleviate it (Norris, 2013). In common understanding, compassion is the desire to alleviate another’s suffering (Moreno-Jiménez, Demerouti, Blanco-Donoso, 2022).

Compassion evolved as a function facilitating cooperation, as well to protect the weak and those suffering. As such, it shapes moral judgment and action (Goetz, Keltner, Simon-Thomas, 2010) in organizations and businesses, as experiencing compassion in times of suffering helps employees resume or reengage with their work (Lilius et al., 2013).

Empathy and Compassion in Changemaking

Empathy requires imagining the role of another (Håkansson, 2003), and as such, is perceived as one of the core drivers of changemaking (Gerdes, Segal, 2011; van Kirk, 2016; Raber, 2018), especially in the field of clinical psychology (e.g., Ickes, 2009).

Compassion is seen as a cardinal component in various areas of diverse human activities (e.g., Davis, 2017), including social entrepreneurship (Grimes et al., 2012; Miller et al., 2012), medicine (e.g., Taylor, 1997; Straughair, 2019), and management (Koopmans, 2018).

Measuring Empathy and Compassion

Measuring Empathy. There have been several studies measuring empathy, dating back as far as the 1940s (Dymond, 1949). A questionnaire developed for social workers, rooted in social cognitive neuroscience and developmental psychology, was introduced as the Empathy Assessment Index (EAI; Gerdes, Segal, 2011). In clinical psychology, there are several instruments available for measuring empathy (Ilgunaite, Giromini, Di Girolamo, 2017). One of the most popular assessment tools is the Empathy Quotient, introduced for individuals with ASD (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, 2004). This article follows on from its concise version, i.e., the Empathy Quotient Short Questionnaire (22 items), which has good psychometric properties (Wakabayashi et al., 2006).

Measuring Compassion. A study analyzing the existing compassion assessment tools identified an unmet need for a psychometrically validated instrument that comprehensively measures the construct of compassion (Sinclair et al., 2017).

This need seems to have been currently met by the recently published Compassion Scale (16 items), which was verified on an N = 465 sample, demonstrating good psychometric properties (Pommier, Neff, Tóth-Király, 2020). Therefore, the Compassion Scale was further used in this article.

Empassion: Merging Empathy with Compassion

On the one hand, empathy is commonly seen as a prosocial and morally positive factor; however, it may also serve as a tool for pursuing negative acts, e.g., through narcissistic or manipulative use of the deepened knowledge of another (Konrath et al., 2014; Breithaupt, 2018). It may especially relate to persons with a psychopathic personality (Hart, 2020; van Dongen, 2020).

This indicates that empathy per se is not enough to assure caring relationships. The complementary parameter should hence relate to sympathizing with others, i.e., having a high compassion level.

Similarly, compassion per se may be blind without understanding the real needs of another and may even turn out to be dangerous (Wel, 2020)—and as such, is seen in some cases as “idiotic compassion” (e.g., McCaffrey, 2015).

The conjecture is that if empathy does not guarantee positive action, and if compassion without empathy may lead to action unrelated to real needs, there emerges a need for a blended phenomenon: Empassion. Recent neuroscience research posits that merging compassion with empathy may be considered a predictor of individuals’ prosocial behavior (Chierchia, Singer, 2017; Stevens, Woodruff, 2018; Stevens, Taber, 2021).

This would require adapting the two scales into a joint one. This Empassion Scale (ES) could then measure the propensity for feeling and understanding others, as well as for caring for them and sympathizing with their problems.

Combining empathy and compassion into one phenomenon and scale provides a comprehensive psychological category allowing to group all of the positive and avoid all of the negative aspects of each category occurring separately.

Research Goals and Hypotheses

The goal of the present study was to validate the new Empassion Scale and verify its validity in comparison to the Empathy Quotient Short and Compassion Scale. Moreover, it aimed to compare the level of Empassion among various segments of the sample.

The main hypothesis is that the new ES Scale will have good psychometric properties, and that its correlation with the Empathy Quotient Short and Compassion Scale will document its validity. A further conjecture is that the Empassion level will be higher in females than males, as well as higher among those with leadership, social activity, and innovativeness experience than those without these experiences.

Measuring Empassion

The inspiration for building a new scale that merges empathy and compassion came from the aforementioned Empathy Quotient Short Questionnaire and Compassion Scale. The 16 initial items for the Empassion Scale were developed from scratch, though inspired by these two existing scales.

Constructing the Empassion Scale (ES)

The conjecture is that there are two primary categories for Empassion: Tuning in with understanding and Engagement. The first is hypothetically broken down into Understanding and Tuning in, while the latter is divided into Emotional engagement and caring for others (see Table 1).

Table 1. Categories, subcategories, and items for the Empassion Scale

Results 1.
Validation of the Empassion Scale

The items mentioned in Table 1 were randomized: four were reversed and four additional buffer items were added.

The Sample. This study was conducted on an N = 338 sample of Polish society, comprising 203 women (60.1%) and 135 men (39.9%); 113 subjects with a leadership role (33.4%) and 225 without (66.6%); 121 subjects involved in a social project (35.8.0%) and 217 not (64.2%); 106 subjects who perceived themselves as innovators (31.4%) and 232 that did not (68.6%).

For age and education, see Tables 2 and 3.

Table 2. Age distribution

Table 3. Education level

Reliability. After removing the buffer items, the analysis showed that the over-all reliability of the 16-item ES was very good: The scale’s Cronbach’s alpha = .891. Table 4 demonstrates the psychometric properties: Cronbach’s alpha for all items was > .8. Moreover, all items, except the four reversed items, correlated with the entire scale between the average and high levels.

Table 4. Psychometric properties of the ES

Factor Analysis. To verify the validity of the questionnaire, factor analysis using the 16 variables of the Empassion scale was conducted. Principal component analysis (PSA) was applied, and a varimax rotation was conducted. The factor analysis method was justified since the Kaiser–Mayer–Olkin test on the standardized data showed a KMO of .92. Additionally, the Bartlett’s test of sphericity was found to be statistically significant (χ²(190) = 3415.689; p < .001). Three factors were identified, explaining 64% of the variance (Table 5).

The significant loadings are shown in bold. This distribution indicates that Factor 1 represents the “Tuning in with understanding” category, while Factor 2 the “Engagement” category (see Table 1). Factor 3 consists solely of reversed items, encompassing all four of them; if reversed back, these four items would also fit the conjected distribution (Table 1).

The reliability of these two factors measured separately turned out to be very high: For Tuning in with understanding, Cronbach’s alpha was .817, and for Engagement, it was .805 (see Tables 6 and 7).

Table 5. Factor analysis: Matrix of the rotated loadings, by varimax rotation

Table 6. Reliability of the Tuning in with understanding factor

Table 7. Reliability of the Engagement factor

Results 2.
Cross-Segment Comparative Analysis

In order to explore the Empassion level in various populations, cross-segment comparative analysis was applied. There was a significant difference in the level of Empassion in all tested groups:

Gender. Empassion in the female group (Nf = 203) was 60.8; meanwhile, in the male group (Nm = 135), it was significantly lower: 55.8; t(336) = 5.58; p = .0000.

Leadership Experience. The Empassion level of those who had leadership experience (NL = 113) differed significantly from those who did not (N~L = 225). In the “No” group Empassion was 5.6, while in the “Yes” group, it was 61.2; t(336) = 3.82; p = .0002.

Social Activity. Those involved in social activities (NS = 121) demonstrated a significantly different empathy level than those who were not (N~S = 217): The “No” group’s empathy level was 57.73, whereas the “Yes” group’s was 60.68; t(336) = 3.13; p = .0019.

Being an Innovator. Empassion of the innovators (NI = 106) significantly differed to that of the noninnovators (N~I = 232): 60.60 vs. 57.95, respectively; t(336) = 2.71; p = .0071.

Results 3.
Validity: Correlation with Empathy Quotient Short and Compassion Scale

To verify the hypothesis that there is a significant correlation between Empassion and empathy, the Pearson’s r (PCC) test was performed, which documented that this correlation was significant (see Tables 8 and 9).

Table 8. Empassion correlation with empathy, coefficients in the entire sample

Table 9. Empassion correlation with compassion, coefficients in the entire sample

This PCC analysis confirmed the validity of the Empassion Scale (ES), documenting that there was a highly significant correlation between ES and both scales: Measuring empathy and compassion.

Conclusions

This study supported the concept of creating a new method of social analysis,

i.e., the Empassion Scale, which proved to have good psychometric properties, being ready to use in further psychological studies.

The theoretical analysis supported the claim that the Empassion phenomenon has a significant meaning in the psychology and social arena and is worth developing as a separate theoretical category, as well as a way of measuring.

This fills the gap, as in various fields of psychology, studies and education are usually conducted separately for empathy and compassion. For example, in business, empathy (e.g., Cohen, 2012; Holt et al., 2017) is detached from compassion (Bejou, 2011; Solomon, 2015), while in healthcare, the approach to empathy (e.g., Sanchez et al., 2019; Moudatsou et al., 2020) is separate and unrelated to compassion (e.g., Youngson, 2011; de Zulueta, 2013; Hojat et al., 2013, 2018). Similarly, in family studies, empathy (Miklikowska, Duriez, Soenens, 2011; Yoo, Feng, Day, 2013) is disconnected from compassion (Park, Ackerman, 2011; Kirby, 2016). This study paves the way for perceiving empathy and compassion as a single positive phenomenon: Empassion, without isolated empathy and compassion doubts.

This study fully supported the conjecture that the Empassion Scale has very good psychometric properties and is significantly correlated with empathy and compassion scales. As such, it is a valuable tool to measure the level of blended empathy and compassion. Moreover, it documented that females scored significantly higher on Empassion than males, similarly to people socially involved, with people who are leaders and innovators having a higher Empassion level than those who are not.

Future studies should confirm the Empassion Scale’s psychometric properties in different (e.g., Great Britain and the USA) and larger samples, using similar and complementary alternative analytic methods.

Empassion may be used as a gateway for training young leaders, as well as caregivers. The ES may help to evaluate training results or to recruit staff.

Training

It is important to add that Empassion is trainable and can be trained and ingrained, especially among youth and future social and business leaders (Chierchia, Singer, 2017; Marsh, 2018). Moreover, leaders can foster conditions that facilitate empathy and compassion (Lilius et al., 2013). The ES may help in appraising the training results.

Conflict Prevention

Empathy is one of the pivotal concepts (together with trust and dialogue) contributing to conflict prevention (Head, 2012). Similarly, compassion and its training could reduce intergroup conflict (Branje, Meeus, 2006; Klimecki, 2019). The blended Empassion Scale may be key to selecting and evaluating peacemakers and peacebuilders in conflicted areas.

Empassion as a Key to Prosocial Behavior: Example of Implementation

The following example demonstrates how Empassion is involved in ingraining prosocial attitudes and behavior:

Mary Gordon,2 based in Canada, is eradicating aggression from schools through bringing neighborhood babies into the classroom in a cycle of “empathy lessons.” She realized that a lack of empathy is at the heart of the growing aggression in primary schools, and thus initiated a way to augment empathy and compassion through introducing classes in which students relate with babies and their mothers into the curriculum. In this vein, she developed the program Roots of Empathy (Gordon, 2005). Longitudinal research, in which classes participating in ROE were matched with similar classes that were not, has shown that children who participated in the program demonstrated, a few years later, decreased aggression and increased prosocial behavior (e.g., sharing, helping, and including) as compared to those who did not (Santos et al., 2011; Schonert-Reichl et al., 2012).

As such, the Roots of Empathy program has been adapted and is spreading throughout Canadian schools, as well as across some European countries.

2 See: https://rootsofempathy.org/. Accessed 30 April 2023.

References

Barnes, J. (Ed.), (2014). Aristotle’s Ethics: Writings from the Complete Works. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Barnett, G., & Mann, R.E. (2013). Empathy deficits and sexual offending: A model of obstacles to empathy. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 18(2), 228–239, doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2012.11.010

Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger’s syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 163–175, doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00

Batson, C.D., Ahmad, N., Lishner, D.A., & Tsang, J. (2005). Empathy and Altruism. In C.R. Snyder, & S.J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of Positive Psychology (pp. 485–498). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Bejou, D. (2011). Compassion as the new philosophy of business. Journal of Relationship Marketing, 10(1), 1–6, doi: 10.1080/15332667.2011.550098

Branje, S.J.T., & Meeus, W.H.J. (2006). Empathy and conflict resolution in friendship relations among adolescents. Aggressive Behavior, 33(1), 48–55.

Breithaupt, F. (2018). The bad things we do because of empathy. Interdisciplinary Science Reviews, 43(2), 166–174, doi: 10.1080/03080188.2018.1450928

Breyer, T. (2020). Empathy, Sympathy and Compassion. In T. Szanto, & H. Landweer (Eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Phenomenology of Emotion (pp. 429–440). New York, NY: Routledge.

Chierchia, G., & Singer, T. (2017). The Neuroscience of Compassion and Empathy and Their Link to Prosocial Motivation and Behavior. In J.C. Dreher, & L. Tremblay (Eds.), Decision Neuroscience (pp. 247–257). Amsterdam: Elsevier, doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-805308-9.00020-8

Cohen, D., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy in conduct-disordered and comparison youth. Developmental Psychology, 32(6), 988–998, doi: 10.1037/0012–1649.32.6.988

Cohen, M.A. (2012). Empathy in business ethics education. Journal of Business Ethics Education, 9, 359–375, doi: 10.5840/jbee2012918

Cuff, B.M.P., Brown, S.J., Taylor, R., & Howat, D.J. (2014). Empathy: A review of the concept. Emotion Review, 8(2), 144–153, doi: 10.1177/1754073914558466

Curzer, H.J. (2007). Aristotle: Founder of the ethics of care. The Journal of Value Inquiry, 41(2–4), 221–243, doi: 10.1007/s10790-007-9088-2

Davis, M.H. (2006). Empathy. In J.E. Stets, & J.H. Turner (Eds.), Handbook of the Sociology of Emotions (pp. 443–466). Boston, MA: Springer, doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-30715-2_20

Davis, M.H. (2017). Empathy, Compassion, and Social Relationships. In E.M. Seppälä, E. Simon-Thomas, S.L. Brown, M.C. Worline, C.D. Cameron, & J.R. Doty (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Compassion Science (pp. 299–316). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

De Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when, and why?

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(10), 435–441, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.008 de Zulueta, P. (2013). Compassion in healthcare. Clinical Ethics, 8(4), doi: 10.1177/ 1477750913506484

Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. The Scientific World Journal, 6, 1146–1163, doi: 10.1100/tsw.2006.221

Decety, J., & Moriguchi, Y. (2007). The empathic brain and its dysfunction in psychiatric populations: Implications for intervention across different clinical conditions. BioPsychoSocial Medicine, 1(22), 1–21, doi: 10.1186/1751-0759-1-22

Dewey, J. (1966). Democracy and Education: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Education. New York: Free Press.

Dymond, R.F. (1949). A scale for the measurement of empathic ability. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 13(2), 127–133, doi: 10.1037/h0061728

Eisenberg, N., & Strayer, J. (1987). Critical Issues in the Study of Empathy. In Empathy and its Development (pp. 3–13). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R.A., & Spinrad, T.L. (2006). Prosocial development. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R.M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3: Social, Emotional and Personality Development (pp. 646–718). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Gerdes, K.E., & Segal, E. (2011). Importance of empathy for social work practice. Social Work, 56(2), 141–148, doi: 10.1093/sw/56.2.141

Goetz, J.L., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: An evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(3), 351–374, doi: 10.1037/a0018807

Gordon, M. (2005). Roots of Empathy. Changing the World Child by Child. Toronto: Thomas Allen Publishers.

Grimes, M.G., McMullen, J.S., Vogus, T.J., & Miller, T.L. (2012). Studying the origins of social entrepreneurship: Compassion and the role of embedded agency. Academy of Management Review, 38(3), 460–463, doi: 10.5465/amr.2012.0429 Håkansson, J. (2003). Exploring the Phenomenon of Empathy. Doctoral dissertation. Stockholm: Department of Psychology, Stockholm University. Retrieved 17 May 2022 from: https://www.atu.dk/sites/default/files/aktiviteter/empathy_ dissertation.pdf

Hall, J.A., & Schwartz, R. (2017). Empathy present and future. The Journal of Social Psychology, 159(3), 225–243, doi: 10.1080/00224545.2018.1477442

Hart, C.L. (2020). Is Empathy the Key to Deception? Psychology Today. Retrieved 17 May 2020 from: https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/the-nature-de-ception/202002/is-empathy-the-key-deception

Head, N. (2012). Transforming conflict: Trust, empathy, and dialogue. International Journal of Peace Studies, 17(2), 33–55.

Hein, G., & Singer, T. (2008). I feel how you feel but not always: The empathic brain and its modulation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 153–158, doi: 10.1016/j. conb.2008.07.012

Hojat, M., DeSantis, J., Shannon, S.C., Mortensen, L.H., Speicher, M.R., Bragan, L., LaNoue, M., & Calabrese, L.H. (2018). The Jefferson Scale of Empathy: A nationwide study of measurement properties, underlying components, latent variable structure, and national norms in medical students. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 23, 899–920, doi: 10.1007/s10459-018-9839-9

Hojat, M., Louis, D.Z., Maio, V., & Gonnella, J.S. (2013). Empathy and health care quality. American Journal of Medical Quality, 28(1), doi: 10.1177/1062860612464731

Holt, S., Marques, J., Hu, J., & Wood, A. (2017). Cultivating empathy, new perspectives on educating business leaders. The Journal of Values-Based Leadership, 10(1), doi: 10.22543/0733.101.1173

Hublin, J.J. (2009). The prehistory of compassion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), 106(16), 6429–6430, doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902614106 Ickes, W. (1997). Empathic Accuracy. New York: Guilford Press.

Ickes, W. (2009). Empathic Accuracy: Its Links to Clinical, Cognitive, Developmental, Social and Physiological Psychology. In J. Decety, & W. Ickes, The Social Neuroscience of Empathy (pp. 57–70). Cambridge, MA: A Bradford Book.

Ilgunaite, G., Giromini, L., & Di Girolamo, M. (2017). Measuring empathy: A literature review of available tools. Applied Psychology Bulletin, 280(65), 2–28.

Kirby, J.N. (2016). The role of mindfulness and compassion in enhancing nurturing family environments. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 23(2), 142–157, doi: 10.1111/cpsp.12149

Klimecki, O.M. (2019). The role of empathy and compassion in conflict resolution. Emotion Review, 11(4), 310–325.

Konrath, S., Corneille, O., Bushman, B.J., & Luminet, O. (2014). The relationship between narcissistic exploitativeness, dispositional empathy, and emotion recognition abilities. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 38(1), 129–143, doi: 10.1007/s10919-013-0164-y

Koopmans, J. (2018). The Power of Compassion in Change Management. LinkedIn. Retrieved 17 May 2020 from: https://www.linkedin.com/pulse/power-compas-sion-change-management-johanna-koopmans/

Lazarus, R.S. (1994). Emotion and Adaptation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Lilius, J.M., Kanov, J., Dutton, J.E., Worline, M.C., & Maitlis, S. (2013). Compassion Revealed. Michigan Ross School of Business, Executive White Papers Series. Retrieved 25 July 2022 from: https://positiveorgs.bus.umich.edu/wp-content/up-loads/Dutton-CompassionRevealed.pdf

Lipps, T. (1903). Einfühlung, inner Nachahmung, und Organumpfindungen (Empathy, inner imitations, and sensations). Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 2, 185–204.

Marsh, A.B. (2018). The neuroscience of empathy. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 110–115, doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.12.016

McCaffrey, G. (2015). Idiot Compassion. In D.W. Jardine, C. Gilham, & G. McCaffrey (Eds.), On the Pedagogy of Suffering (pp. 19–27). New York: Peter Lang Inc.

Mead, G.H. (1967). Mind, Self, and Society: From the Standpoint of a Social Behaviorist. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Miklikowska, M., Duriez, B., & Soenens, B. (2011). Family roots of empathy-related characteristics: The role of perceived maternal and paternal need support in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1342–1352, doi: 10.1037/a0024726

Miller, T.L., Grimes, M.G., McMullen, J.S., & Vogus, T.J. (2012). Venturing for others with heart and head: How compassion encourages social entrepreneurship.

Academy of Management Review, 37(4), 616–640, doi: 10.5465/amr.2010.0456

Moreno-Jiménez, J.E., Demerouti, E., Blanco-Donoso, L.M. (2022). Passionate health-care workers in demanding intensive care units: Its relationship with daily exhaustion, secondary traumatic stress, empathy, and self-compassion. Current Psychology, doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03986-z

Morrison, K.F. (2013). Framing the subject: Humanity and the wounds of love. Studies on medieval empathies. Brepols, 1, 1–58, doi: 10.1484/M.DISPUT-EB.1.100304

Moudatsou, M., Stavropoulou, A., Philalithis, A., & Koukouli, S. (2020). The role of empathy in health and social care professionals. Healthcare, 8(1), 26, doi: 10.3390/ healthcare8010026

Norris, K. (2013). The development and evaluation of a compassion scale. Journal of Health Care Poor Underserved, 24(3), 1235–1246.

Oliveira-Silva, P., & Gonçalves, O.F. (2011). Responding empathically: A question of heart, not a question of skin. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 36(6), 201–207, doi: 10.1007/s10484-011-9161-2

Park, J.H., & Ackerman, J.M. (2011). Passion and Compassion: Psychology of Kin Relations Within and Beyond the Family. In C. Salmon, & T.K. Shackleford (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Family Psychology (pp. 329–344). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Piaget, J. (2008). The Moral Judgment of the Child. London: Free Press Paperbacks.

Pommier, E., Neff, K.D., & Tóth-Király, I. (2020). The development and validation of the Compassion Scale. Assessment, 27(1), 21–39, doi: 10.1177/1073191119874108

Praszkier, R. (2014). Empathy, mirror neurons and SYNC. Mind & Society, 15(1), 1–25, doi: 10.1007/s11299-014-0160-x

Raber, R. (2018). Towards an empathic human rights. Left History, 22(1), 89–110, doi: 10.25071/1913-9632.39369

Read, H. (2019). A typology of empathy and its many moral forms. Philosophy Compass, 14(10), 1–12, doi: 10.1111/phc3.12623

Roy, S. (2010). The Psychology of Empathy. FutureHealth. Retrieved 20 July 2022 from: www.futurehealth.org/populum/page.php?f=The-Psychology-of-Empathy-by-Saberi-Roy-100620-281.html

Ruys, J.F. (2018). An alternative history of medieval empathy: The scholastics and compassion. Emotions: History, Culture, Society, 2(2), 192–213, doi: 10.1163/ 2208522X-02010019

Sanchez, G., Peterson, M.W., Musser, E.D., Galynker, I., Sandhu, S., & Foster, A.E. (2019). Measuring Empathy in Health Care. In A.E. Foster, & Z.S. Yaseen (Eds.), Teaching Empathy in Healthcare (pp. 63–84). Cham: Springer, doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-29876-0_4

Santos, R.G., Chartier, M.J., Whalen, J.C., Chateau, D., & Boyd, L. (2011). Effectiveness of school-based violence prevention for children and youth: Cluster randomized field trial of the Roots of Empathy program with replication and three-year follow-up. Healthcare Quarterly, 14(2), 80–90, doi: 10.12927/hcq.2011.22367

Schonert-Reichl, K.A., Smith, V., Zaidman-Zait, A., & Hertzman, C. (2012). Promoting children’s prosocial behaviours in school: Impact of the “Roots of Empathy” program on the social and emotional competence of schoolaged children. School Mental Health, 4(1), 1–12, doi: 10.1007/s12310-011-9064-7

Sinclair, S., Russell, L.B., Hack, T.F., Kondejewski, J., & Sawatzky, R. (2017). Measuring compassion in healthcare: A comprehensive and critical review. A Comprehensive and Critical Review. Patient, 10, 389–405, doi: 10.1007/s40271-016-0209-5

Solomon, R.C. (2015). The moral psychology of business: Care and compassion in the corporation. Business Ethics Quarterly, 8(3), 515–533, doi: 10.2307/3857435

Stevens, F., & Taber, K. (2021). The neuroscience of empathy and compassion in pro-social behavior. Neuropsychologia, 159(107925), 3–10, doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107925

Stevens, L.C., & Woodruff, C.C. (2018). What is This Feeling that I Have for Myself and for Others? Contemporary Perspectives on Empathy, Compassion and Self-Compassion, and Their Absence. In L.C. Stevens, & C.C. Woodruff (Eds.), The Neuroscience of Empathy, Compassion, and Self-Compassion (pp. 1–21). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.

Straughair, C. (2019). Cultivating compassion in nursing: A grounded theory study to explore the perceptions of individuals who have experienced nursing care as patients. Nurse Education in Practice, 35(9), 98–103, doi: 10.1016/j.nepr.2019.02.002

Taylor, M.B. (1997). Compassion: Its neglect and importance. British Journal of General Practice, 47(421), 521–523.

Titchener, E.B. (1909). Lectures on the Experimental Psychology of the Thought-Processes. New York: The MacMillan Company.

van der Weele, C. (2011). Empathy’s purity, sympathy’s complexities: De Waal, Darwin and Adam Smith. Biology and Philosophy, 26(4), 583–593, doi: 10.1007/s10539-011-9248-4

van Dongen, J.D.M. (2020). The empathic brain of psychopaths: From social science to neuroscience in empathy. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 695, doi: 10.3389/ fpsyg.2020.00695

van Kirk, G. (2016). Social Entrepreneurship Core Value: It All Starts with Empathy. LinkedIn. Retrieved 16 May 2022 from: https://www.linkedin.com/pulse/social-entrepreneurship-core-value-all-starts-empathy-greg-van-kirk/

Wakabayashi, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., Smith, R., & Weil, L. (2006). Development of short forms of the Empathy Quotient (EQ-Short) and the Systemizing Quotient (SQ-Short). Personality and Individual Differences, 41(5), 929–940, doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.03.017

Wel, M. (2020). Why Blind Compassion Is Dangerous. Psychology Today. Retrieved 28 July 2022 from: https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/urban-survival/202012/why-blind-compassion-is-dangerous

Yoo, H., Feng, X., & Day, R.D. (2013). Adolescents’ empathy and prosocial behavior in the family context: A longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 1858–1872, doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9900-6

Youngson, R. (2011). Compassion in Healthcare—the Missing Dimension of Health-care Reform? In I. Renzenbrink (Ed.), Caregiver Stress and Staff Support in Illness, Dying and Bereavement (pp. 37–48). Oxford: Oxford University Press.